Hacker News new | past | comments | ask | show | jobs | submit login
Glucosamine significantly reduces risk of lung cancer and lung cancer mortality (ersjournals.com)
161 points by birriel on June 14, 2022 | hide | past | favorite | 116 comments



These results from observational studies almost never replicate in randomized studies. It's because people who decide to take glucosamine (or not) also do lots of other confounding things, like visit the doctor more, or have higher salaries, or don't live with people who smoke etc. It's been shown many times you can't really adjust for these things. Epidemiologists love doing these studies, because they are comparatively easy. The hardest part is the data sharing agreement with whatever registry you are using. You don't even need to leave your desk to collect any data.


The real question is: why is anything other than randomised studies allowed to be published in the first place?


Because academics and primary consumers of these studies understand their uses and limitations. Not all science has to be groundbreaking new discoveries, simply trying to replicate an existing study or exact same methodology on different cohort is perfectly fine.

The problem is when it gets printed in mainstream press who sensationalize and then posted to reddit/hn where people go 'duh this is obvious, this is a dumb thing to study', 'sample too small', 'not controlling for income/confounding'.


One answer: anyone can start a journal, there are no laws or standards. Peer reviewers are busy and don't always have the background to criticize every aspect of a paper.

Another: not all research can or should have randomized studies (like it just wouldn't make sense if you were, say, determining the structure of a protein). So drawing the line as to what does and doesn't need a specific approach is blurry, and really, can only be enforced during the peer review process, or by the editor.

On the other hand, this kind of work can still serve as a hypothesis generator. Someone else will read this, think, "well they did this all wrong!" and perform a better study that clearly demonstrates whether it has an effect or not.


I guess it's ok to publish uninteresting and probably false things. This could be a fairly accurate description of biomedical science to be honest. It's annoying when it ends up on HN for example, or in the health section of a tabloid newspaper.

In theory, these observational findings could suggest treatments that should be evaluated in a randomised trial. I am not actually sure this is true however, it is possible they are less useful than selecting interventions for further testing in some other way. Anecdotally, folk remedies may have a higher hit rate for selecting treatments for proper evaluation. This is often surprising - eg a precursor to aspirin is found in the Willow tree, which has been used as a medicine for thousands of years. Another example is the foxglove plant from which the heart drug digoxin is derived, which was used as a herbal medicine predating modern medicine.



> The real question is: why is anything other than randomised studies allowed to be published in the first place?

Because things other than randomized studies produce the results that generate the funding for randomized studies.


Not questioning the need for RCTs, but they have their own issues. Like, let's say you do a RCT and it shows an effect, but then in a large epi observational study there isn't any? There would be some problems to work out.

RCTs are great but can have problems with generalizability to real-world conditions. Ideally you'd study both.


With a sufficiently robust RCT, I'd think that indicates an issue with the observation in the observational study -- i.e., there are some confounding factors in the real world -- rather than any sort of issue with whatever effect the RCT found.

Conversely, if we had a large observational study that was then contradicted by a robust RCT... well, I'd still be inclined to trust the RCT.

(At the same time, I think a good observational study is, by its nature, more applicable to one's own life. If an observational study suggests that $FOOBAR is good, then -- as long as there's also a solid RCT confirming that $FOOBAR is safe -- why not try out some $FOOBAR for yourself?)


Observational studies are far cheaper and easier, and are actually very useful for a lot of situations.


This is a good point. I was recently having a conversation with a non-scientist (I am not one either) about two different studies. One was an RCT that found no effect for a treatment, and the other was a "case control, test-negative" design study that found a beneficial effect. My friend said the latter was as good as an RCT because it also has a control arm.

But when I looked up what "case control, test-negative" means, I found that it is observational, and there is no intervention provided to any of the individuals. Some of the recent discussions of the design indicate that it is understood by scientists, but can mislead laypeople, who assume that its conclusions are much more robust than they actually are.

We should definitely continue to do research of many types, partly as a way to figure out where to spend the time/money to do RCTs (which are more expensive than other types of analysis). But we need people reporting on studies to be very clear up-front when they are not describing an RCT. They should say what the study's conclusion means, versus what it would mean if there were a similar 'finding' in an RCT. Otherwise people will not understand that they are being told a weak conclusion, not realizing there is a strong conclusion that has gone unmentioned.


Published, or publicized?

All science needs to be published. Pilot studies, observational studies, quasi-experimental studies. Otherwise, we don't have the information we need to create randomized control trials.

However, I'd strongly support keeping all that science from being publicized.


> However, I'd strongly support keeping all that science a secret from journalists.

What’s your plan for doing this?


Some kind of secret police force with balaclavas and chairs in dark rooms.


You can't do randomized studies for things like nutrition if the question is "does eating red meat for a decade increase the risk of cancer". Nobody's going to comply with it.


Not everything can be done with a randomized controlled study. For some fields (quantum mechanics, volcanology, climate change, paleontology...) most studies can't be randomized, much less blind. This is also true of longitudinal studies: you can't fully control anyone's exercise and diet over months, much less decades.

Yet we want to do research in these areas. So we have to make do with what's possible.


True! But medicine just so happens to be one of those fields that lends itself well to RCT’s. Perhaps if the OP had reworded their statement to everything in medicine it might make more sense.


some medical trials. Surgeries, for example, are tough. And these longitudinal nutritional studies, as I mentioned, are another.


With all the common sense replies explaining why, I guess this was not the real question at all.


Actually, you're wrong about that. According to this Cochrane systematic review, RCT's and observational studies are usually in close agreement: https://www.cochranelibrary.com/cdsr/doi/10.1002/14651858.MR...


Well no, I’m not wrong. That article you linked is a review of reviews comparing RCTs and observational studies for medical interventions, primarily surgical studies or pharmacological treatments with well characterised effects. Observational studies of supplements and nutritional interventions on the other hand have a very poor track record of RCT validation.


@Gatsky: ah I see, yes that sounds highly plausible, but can you supply any evidence of this, or is it just hearsay?


Such a review is necessarily limited to topics which have both an RCT and an observational study.

As such, the general bias against publishing non-significant results could mean the conditions for being able to even try such a review risk biasing the review towards exaggerating agreement.

(It also seems the Cochrane review here may itself only be considering other reviews - a metameta analysis of sorts – which might also be influenced by a selection bias, if there’s any chance at all that researchers choosing work, & then publication decisions, prefer to address questions where there’s more agreement, rather than less-attention-catching mixed results.)

The most convincing evidence here would be how strongly preregistered RCT results, whether ever published or not, tend to match earlier observational studies. It doesn’t appear to me that this Cochrane review is focused on that.


@gojomo: Isn't it normally the case that an RCT follows an observational study? There's not much point in doing an observational study if someone's already done an RCT, but there is the other way round, since RCT's are more trusted.

Wrt publication bias; an underestimation of the false positive rate of observational studies wrt RCTs could occur if non-significant RCTs are being held back at a higher rate than observational ones, but it seems more likely to me that it's the other way around since RCTs are generally more expensive and time consuming than observational studies, and more trusted, so it's a bigger loss if the result isn't published.


I don’t have a sense of the ‘normal’; it might vary by field.

For example, for COVID vaccines, we’re being inundated with observational data - often informal, sometimes in reviewed studies - long after initial approval RCTs.


Hmm, that would be interesting, except it seems they only considered 14 studies. They find in 11 of these observational studies match the results of RCTs.

Im sure this will be different in areas where there is no public awareness of a risk factor vs ones where there are. Eg healthy diets select for people who care about their health, so it's not so infomative, vs something where the link is not publicly known (so that the observational study is already somewhat blind/randomized).


Good advice in general but it’s worth reading to see what confounders they tried to correct for. There’s still information to be gleaned.

This part caught my eye:

> A stronger association between glucosamine use and decreased lung cancer risk was observed in participants with a family history of lung cancer when compared with those without a family history.


A family history of lung cancer could simply mean a family history of smoking, which was much more common a few generations ago than it is today.


This was done in Guangzhou where I lived for years until 2018. The incidence of smoking amongst males still higher - gut guess of 30-40% adult males are smokers in the city and province. So in their cohort one would expect to be seeing still living relatives of young adult smokers who are also smokers.

Interesting that it’s sold as supplement for joint and related tissue repair and yet first big science study with observable results is about cancer.

Other interesting point is that it’s study of cancer. And that is simply because China still has huge numbers of smokers and the health cost is stratospheric.


Is it actually stratospheric though, when you take out QOL year adjustments? Other studies, such as this one in Finland[1], found that when you don’t take into account QOL adjustments for the smoker that smoking is actually a net benefit for society because the smokers die earlier and don’t draw on the very expensive late life healthcare.

1: https://bmjopen.bmj.com/content/2/6/e001678


To me it is plausible that in a time of great social and economic change, the odds of behavioral confounds in observational studies might increase. Non-stationarity everywhere.

That said, a meaningful result here would be welcome.


You’re right that there is non-random allocation to the exposure (taking glucosamine), but this is something that can be addressed to an extent with a bit of statistics.

I was disappointed to see the authors didn’t take the additional step of doing propensity score matching or weighting to account for propensity to take glucosamine.

Here’s a good reference about propensity score matching: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3144483/


I wonder, does anyone know of a good guide for lay persons that explains how to understand scientific studies?

Even something that details the difference between a population study and randomized controlled trials.


Ben Goldacre's book 'Bad Pharma' has a good discussion around this, especially in terms of caveats and gotchas to watch out for with such studies.

It might be more 'lay person' than you are looking for, but it's an excellent read for myriad other reasons, so you wouldn't be wasting your time (IMO).


Thanks. I was hoping for something online, like a blog post/website, which is an easy way to spread the word to other people so they know how to contextualize these things.


I agree with that. You can see this in Ontario COVID statistics.

People that did not take the vaccine, despite all the pressure, tend to be healthier as population. They kind of self selected by being self confident in their health and not worrying about dying from COVID. The triple vaccinated in Ontario are now the highest risk of getting COVID. They too self selected, by tending to be older with pre existing health problems, and worried enough to get boosters.

https://covid-19.ontario.ca/data?fbclid=1


> The triple vaccinated in Ontario are now the highest risk of getting COVID

I don't understand how the source you shared supports this claim*. If anything, it shows the opposite - you're at far higher risk of death if you're unvaccinated. And that's after considering the fact that unvaccinated people are likely to be younger and fitter.

The unvaccinated have, at best, half as many deaths per 100k as those who are fully vaccinated + booster (in reality, the two lines mostly track each other). Which sounds good, until you realize that the unvaccinated make up only 9% of the population. 33% of the deaths coming from 9% of the population - healthier, they are not. Delusional, would be more correct.

* I'm assuming "getting COVID" here means "having serious consequences from an infection" as opposed to merely being infected. The vaccinated make up the vast majority of the population, so it's obvious that they are also at the highest risk of being infected by COVID. There are very few unvaccinated people left for the virus to infect.


The numbers have already been adjusted. So what you're seeing is the rate of covid cases per 100k people in each group. 3rd graph from the top. You're trying to adjust the numbers twice for the unvaccinated.

If you don't believe me, here is a second set of data from Walgreens. Look at the third page.

https://www.walgreens.com/businesssolutions/covid-19-index.j...

The Ontario page does not show the death rate.


> The numbers have already been adjusted

Thanks for the correction.

> The Ontario page does not show the death rate.

Doesn't it? I see a graph titled "Deaths involving COVID-19 by vaccination status". And that still shows higher deaths/100k in the "Not fully vaccinated" group for every given adult age range: 18-39, 40-59, and 60+ (look at the All time data).

> here is a second set of data from Walgreens. Look at the third page.

That shows the positivity rate. I'm not talking about the positivity rate. We're beyond that now. Everyone will get Covid someday. The vaccine protects you from dying or having serious health issues. There's no vaccine, for any disease, that can prevent you from contracting the disease. That would require vaccines to create force fields around your body, which are science fiction. In the case of the most efficacious vaccines, your immune system will be so well-prepared that your body will fight off the infection without you ever getting any symptoms.


I missed the "Deaths stats" as I did not see those before, the last time I looked.

So the last cited death rate is 0.01 vs 0.02 / per 100,000 people. So the vaccinated have 1 death per (100 * 100,000 = 10 million). Or 1 death per 10 million and the "not fully vaccinated" have 2 deaths per 10 million. To give context Ontario has a population of 14 million people. This seems like an easy relative win for the vaccine.

Except:

1) The numbers of deaths are so low, its kind of meaningless to extrapolate to the general population from them, because of sample bias effects.

One reason being that deaths could be coming from a very likely specific sub population. For example very old sick people already in hospital or nursing homes near death that contract the disease. Pretty much anything could kill them. It has no bearing on how a random person from the general population would react. You might have situation where for example there are people that chemo therapy failed, and they either refuse the vaccine (since they will die within weeks anyway) or might be so sick and too weak to take the vaccine and then contract covid as the last straw that breaks them.

So it would be very disingenuous to claim based on such small number that the vaccine lowered deaths in the general population. It would be like telling people in Hawaii to wear gloves to prevent frostbite, based on data collected from Canada showing that people that did not wear gloves had twice the rate of frostbite.

2) "Unvaccinated" and "Not Fully Vaccinated" are two different things according to their definition. They're clearly counting anyone that died within two weeks of getting a shot as being unvaccinated, even if it was their second shot. This is not a fair comparison.

3) Our prime minister caught covid twice in the last 4 months despite being vaccinated and boosted.

There have been studies that show that natural acquired immunity is longer lasting. Since the odds of dying are so low at this point, would you rather get covid once and feel a little bit sicker, unvaccinated, for longer lasting immunity. Or would you rather get vaccinated and boosted and catch it twice, feeling a little less sick each time. Which is what the data seems to be showing is happening. This seems like it should be personal preference decision.


The Ontario page shows a graph comparing death rates for various states of vaccination search for "Deaths involving COVID-19 by vaccination status".


does that include the people who died after not taking the vaccine?

asking because you only linked to the homepage of a general data portal which itself reports its no longer being updated,

versus to a study supporting what you're saying


Last update was June 10. So it's still pretty recent. Ontario did not release data on death rates to the public. I don't know why. I briefly read on twitter that UK is showing startling differences now, with the vaccinated having a much higher all cause mortality rate. If true, I don't know if its this effect at play or if some of the conspiracy theories were right.


just reiterating in case you missed it, regardless of when it was last updated:

you actually only linked to the homepage of a general data portal (not even a specific data set),

versus to a study supporting what you're saying

I have to imagine this is a mistake in getting the specific URL you were actually looking at


You are right that people who take vaccines and who do not probably bifurcate along other lines as well. But you’re doing everything even more of a disservice by speculating the dimensions as if it’s established fact. Maybe the unvaccinated aren’t healthier they’re just liars? Also any self respecting study would control for the simple demographics like age so your hypothesis would be quite moot if the study was done with any rigor.


[flagged]


It was literally never claimed that it was 97% effective at stopping COVID. The Pfizer vaccine was 97% effective at preventing severe disease after two doses in early 2021. Don't bring conspiracy nonsense into every discussion about healthcare.



Here is the claim from pfizer's own press release.

"Vaccine effectiveness was at least 97% against symptomatic COVID-19 cases, hospitalizations, severe and critical hospitalizations, and deaths. Furthermore, the analysis found a vaccine effectiveness of 94% against asymptomatic SARS-CoV-2 infections. "

https://www.pfizer.com/news/press-release/press-release-deta....


You just proved yourself wrong? What do you think this says? It literally never claims to be 97% effective at (your words) stopping COVID. That implies that it prevents the infection from spreading and/or causing symptoms, neither of which is part of that statement in the press release.


To be very specific they took two samples of about 22,000 people each. One vaccinated and one given placebo. Then they counted how many people they caught with covid in each sample. 8 vs 162. Then based on those numbers claimed the 97% effectiveness as stopping infection (people getting sick).

Here's a video confirming that this is how they measured efficiency.

https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=huFLxbBw9OM

8 vs 162 is the people that they confirmed with PCR test to be positive. They did not actually test everyone for covid. They admit that there were other people they suspected of having covid.

"Among 3210 total cases of suspected but unconfirmed COVID-19 in the overall study population, 1594 occurred in the vaccine group vs 1816 in the placebo group." Page 42 of the pfizer data leaks.

Hope you see why claiming 97% efficiency to the public was misleading.

After a year, and millions of vaccines later, we can clearly say that the study was bullshit. The vaccine did not offer anywhere near the sort of long lasting immunity that approaches 97%.

Our prime minster despite having two shots and boosted was sick with covid twice in 4 months.

So to cite this study as being accurate is kind of like claiming pigs can fly. Yes I suppose they could. If they jump off a cliff, for a brief second they could.


Again, you are completely wrong and simply spouting vaccine conspiracy nonsense. It is very clear from the study that those "suspected but unconfirmed COVID-19 cases" are those who reported one flu-like symptom but had a NEGATIVE PCR test. A very detailed explanation of why you are wrong to want to include those in the efficacy percentage number can be found here: https://www.covid-datascience.com/post/refuting-peter-doshi-.... It is not clear whatsoever that the study was bullshit, and the results speak for themselves. An anecdote about your Prime Minister is meaningless and occurred AFTER this initial study period when the variants had changed the landscape and these numbers were no longer applicable.


Since you are still claiming that it was 95% effective. Can you give a time estimate for how long that efficiency lasted?

This study concludes " Primary immunization with two doses of ChAdOx1 nCoV-19 or BNT162b2 vaccine provided limited protection against symptomatic disease caused by the omicron variant. "

https://www.nejm.org/doi/full/10.1056/NEJMoa2119451#:~:text=....

Why are they wrong?

I read the article you cited. Here's the problem as I see with it.

"Among 3210 total cases of suspected but unconfirmed COVID-19 in the overall study population, 1594 occurred in the vaccine group vs 1816 in the placebo group"

The pfizer results are dependent on the accuracy of the PCR test.

"The false-negative rate for SARS-CoV-2 RT-PCR testing is highly variable: highest within the first 5 days after exposure (up to 67%), and lowest on day 8 after exposure (21%)."

https://www.acc.org/latest-in-cardiology/journal-scans/2020/...

1. Even if the PCR test FNR was just 2% then some covid positive people were missed that need to be accounted for.

1594 * 0.02 = 31 were false negative in the vaccinated group

1816 * 0.02 = 36 were false negative in the unvaccinated group.

8 + 31 = 41

162 + 36 = 198

41 / 198 = 20% => Then the pfizer vaccine efficiency falls to 80

If the PCR test FNR was 10% Then we need to add 159 and 181 respectively

8 + 159 = 167

162 + 181 = 343

167/343 = 48%. Efficiency falls to 52%

Real world FNR for a PCR test is estimated to be between 2-30%, but can be much higher depending on which day you get tested.

https://www.gavi.org/vaccineswork/false-negative-how-long-do...

https://www.acc.org/latest-in-cardiology/journal-scans/2020/...

2. PCR tests do not show previous infection. Meaning that Pfizer PCR testing would miss all those people that had "flu-like symptoms" had covid, and recovered prior to getting tested. Did the pfizer study accounts for such likely scenarios?

Finally, why were there so many people with "Flu-like symptoms" in the pfizer study yet they did not have covid or the flu? Since we know that flu infections were at record lows during the last two years. Any explanation for this?


95% not 97%


That's 94%, and it was true vs the original strain immediately after vaccination. Now many people are six months or more past their last shot and the current virus is two years of evolution improved vs the original. Vaccination reduces personal risk significantly but it's not going to control spread by itself.


Glucosamine is both a precursor metabolite and major component of complex carbohydrates in the cell.

Glucosamine (GlcN) gets acetylated to form GlcNAc, which is found in pretty much all the complex carbohydrate types in humans. It’s also the main component of the chitin shells on our insect/crustacean friends.

You can also find GlcN in glycosaminoglycans (GAGs), in heparan sulfates (transformed from GlcNAc to an N-sulfated GlcN by the NDSTs) as a prerequisite for further sulfation. The pattern of sulfation on these sugars is important for determining the bioactivity and interactions for each of the GAGs. For example, the sulfation pattern on heprarin is important to tune to avoid HITT while still maintaining anticoagulant activity.


Any concerns about glucosamine raising eye pressure? And also, if NAG is a component of GAGs, doesn’t that mean taking an NAG supplement could contribute towards increased ocular pressure as well?

Or are the chemical pathways in the body too dissimilar for that to happen?


If there’s a connection, it might be via hyaluronan, another glycan polymer, which is abundant in the vitreous humor in the eye. I don’t know much about how it’s regulated.

Generally, I don’t think we know enough about how metabolite levels translate to specific complex carb levels because there’s a relatively complex set of intermediate steps which themselves can be regulated.


There have been other studies with similar results but the main drawback of these population analyses is that glucosamine use is more likely in people with healthy lifestyles.

Other supplements have shown benefits in these sorts of studies without standing up to more exacting experiment design.

So potentially interesting results but nothing worth getting invested in.


Related, glucosamine treats had a quick positive impact on the mobility of my aging dog. I recommend these: https://stratfordrx.com/products/max-strength-joint-support-...


The problem with glucosamine is that it reliably increases intraocular pressure.

https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5350357/


Is there research to engineer ways so that we can use blood as delivery only in some areas ? I know it sounds stupid but maybe toying with timings so that chemicals never live long enough to reach upper body / brain ? or maybe microcapsules that only dissolve in certain biological environment ?


Specifically targeting compounds is an active area of research. Example: all oncotherapies are essentially just poison. The trick is to deliver the toxin to the 0.001% of cells in the body that are cancerous.

It is amusing you should say ways that avoid reaching the brain. The blood-brain barrier impedes a large number of therapeutics from entering the brain. It is incredibly difficult to engineer the constraints of something that: is potent enough to have an effect, can survive in the blood, cross into the brain, targets the cells of interest, has a meaningful half-life, does not decompose into something toxic, etc.


Does it raise it enough to cause issues? What are the issues associated with increased pressure?



Literally the first Google result for "eye pressure" is about glaucoma. So go figure.


Interesting. I‘ve been taking a glucosamine supplement for several years since I saw a study that correlates it with overall reduced mortality.


Me too. I’m still here!


Confirmation bias?


Survivorship bias.


more like survivorship bias?


Perhaps the causality here is that if you care about reducing own mortality, then your general course is action ends up reducing your mortality. It would be difficult to tell if it's glucosamine that's the cause, or some other thing a person of the kind that takes glucosamine would do (or all of such things in combination).


I can’t speak for people, but I did notice meaningful improvement in my dog’s joint pain when we provided glucosamine supplements.

Inflammation is correlated with many conditions, so I would not be surprised to see other correlations, or that it was combined with other inflammation reducing agents.


I wouldn't argue that it might be helpful, who knows, just the study in question does not rule out what I said and neither does anecdotal evidence


I was taking it, but then I died


I'm unfamiliar with glucosamine aside from the quick search and comments I read now. Neat that they see a correlation, but are there any theories about why this would be the case? Do the lungs use glucosamine somehow?


Anecdotal, but I had a conversation about smoking with a med science professor many years ago. He said something along the lines of “as long as you don’t eat seafood your risk is low”. I was baffled.. looks like there is some truth to it.


Seafood is a main source of glucosamine so shouldn't it be "as long as you DO eat seafood your risk is low"?


The risk of what is low?


Based on the title and reference to smoking, I imagine he means the risk of lung cancer.


According to a google search, "the only natural food sources are shellfish shells from shrimp, lobster and crabs". So I wonder if you can see a significant effect in people who eat a lot of shrimp.


Interesting, I'm vegan and the glucosamine I've ordered has claimed to be sourced from a fungus. Perhaps the fungus isn't edible (hence, it's not a natural food source)


The fungus probably isn't normally eaten. Shrimp shells aren't exactly a treat.


Well if it is truly only in the shells, then probably not!


A lot of shrimp recipes have you cook the shells in oil for extra flavor. This one is a personal favorite:

https://www.seriouseats.com/spanish-style-garlic-shrimp-gamb...

Seriously. Get some good shrimp, some garlic, and go to town on some crusty bread. It's one of the recipes I'd still eat even if it took time off my life, it's just so damn good.


My favorite sushi restaurant in San Mateo, CA (sushi sams) would serve a single fried shrimp appetizer with the head still on. It's a crispy tasty delight!


also, the really small shrimp are just cooked and eaten with the shell


Eating shrimp shell-on and fried is fairly common in a lot of countries


at least the small ones are very tasty (& crunchy) eaten this way


Well I’m vegan but that is good to know thanks.


It would be cool if someone made a dashboard of studies complete with their listed interventions to find out which were the most effective for preventing cancer and reducing cancer mortality.


Not quite what you asked for, but this is a nice resource for at a glance assessment of supplements:

https://www.informationisbeautiful.net/visualizations/snake-...


This is great. Thank you very much for the recommendation.


examine.com has a lot of good studies referenced.


Glucosamine is taken a lot by runners as its promoted as a suppliment that helps cartilidge in the knees. Runners are unlikely to be current smokers and more likely to be working their lungs in a healhty way.


If you are allergic to shellfish you are going to have a fun time finding out with conventional glucosamine.

Note there is a version made from corn but more expensive and you have to look for it (I think it's called "GreenGrown"). I think it's chemically identical?


I love when you see such a huge study yield significant results.


Interesting study, but it needs replication. A 15% reduction in cancer incidence is a rather large reduction, assuming this result is replicated in a different, ideally non-European cohort.


It had almost half a million subjects. Isn't that enough replication?


Shouldn't we want replication even if the sample size seems large, to rule out experimental errors or other flaws in the work?


It isn't the most diverse cohort so a body such as the WHO might or might not choose to make recommendations based on this single finding.


This is true. That’s a very large effect.

But as others have stated it’s observational, not an interventional study. Thus less rigorous.



Murders By Steam....


This is hardly the first time that Glucosamine use is associated with lower cancer/mortality risks:

https://ard.bmj.com/content/79/6/829 - Associations of regular glucosamine use with all-cause and cause-specific mortality: a large prospective cohort study

https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/33219063/ - Glucosamine/Chondroitin and Mortality in a US NHANES Cohort

https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3175750/ - Use of Glucosamine and Chondroitin and Lung Cancer Risk in the VITamins And Lifestyle (VITAL) Cohort

https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5870876/ - Glucosamine Use and Risk of Colorectal Cancer: Results from the Cancer Prevention Study II Nutrition Cohort

https://cancerci.biomedcentral.com/articles/10.1186/1475-286... - Glucosamine suppresses proliferation of human prostate carcinoma DU145 cells through inhibition of STAT3 signaling (in vitro)

https://www.pharmacytoday.org/article/S1042-0991(17)30735-1/... - Glucosamine and chondroitin decrease colorectal cancer risk

Some of those still showed significant correlation even when accounting for NSAID use however there have been other studies where the correlation significance disappears when accounting for NSAIDs: https://www.nature.com/articles/s41598-018-20349-6

Mortality reduction with glucosamine has been directly shown in more controlled studies on other organisms such as nematodes and mice, but may do so through a path that hasn't been shown to work in humans: https://www.nature.com/articles/ncomms4563

And in an actual double blind study glucosamine (+ chondroitin) use was found to lower serum levels of C-reactive protein in men and women: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/article?id=10.1371/journal... (small sample size)


I find it difficult to believe that it has "significant" health benefits. Glucosamine has never shown significant health benefits, only minor ones: https://examine.com/supplements/glucosamine/

I find it extremely difficult to trust an all-Chinese research team on a substance heavily used in "traditional chinese medicine", for two reasons:

"TCM" is the invention of Mao, who needed to look like he was doing something to provide medical care for billions: https://slate.com/technology/2013/10/traditional-chinese-med...

...and the CCP has been pushing it very heavily: https://www.economist.com/leaders/2017/08/31/china-is-rampin...

Or more simply put, it's utter tripe that was passed off to peasants to keep them from rioting over having essentially no healthcare system.


> Glucosamine has never shown significant health benefits, only minor ones

... This is a bit of a ridiculous argument to make when looking at the data. First of all, they only have 8 categories, only 2 of which have more than 2 studies cited. Second of all, imagine if you only studied the effects of aspirin on addiction. I'm assuming you'd find little to no effect. And if Examine had a list of addiction-related effects you'd also be like "look aspirin is weak, how can it suddenly be strong?". The listed effects on examine all have to do with pain relief which likely has little in common with the mechanism of action for lung cancer

> I find it extremely difficult to trust an all-Chinese research team

This is published in a (European) journal with an H-index of 255.[0] The journal is reputable so are you just asserting you can't trust Chinese scientists to study ingredients used in TCM?

> "TCM" is the invention of Mao

This is very false. Even the article you link only says Mao "heavily invested" in it. You know who else is heavily investing in traditional medicine? The World Health Organization. Which currently has a decades long plan to support and fund and increase the role that complementary medicines play in non-industrialized countries[1][2]

[0] https://www.scimagojr.com/journalsearch.php?q=18458&tip=sid&...

[1] https://www.who.int/health-topics/traditional-complementary-...

[2] https://www.who.int/publications/i/item/9789241506096


> You know who else is heavily investing in traditional medicine? The World Health Organization

That's not the best argument if you are trying to debunk CCP conspiracies: https://www.bbc.com/news/world-asia-52088167


Sorry I'm not really following. Yeah Taiwan's treatment is problematic but that's a larger issue with the United Nations as a whole and not something specific to WHO...


Taiwan's treatment is a result of pressure from China, which the WHO, uniquely or not, apparently kowtows to.


Your concerns are valid. The IPCC reports were also heavily criticized for heavy biases in due to pressure from the US (particularly the US military). However, that doesn't mean that the IPCC reports weren't overall an incredibly useful scientific feat

Yes, like the IPCC, WHO is also susceptible to international political pressures, but I really think if you read through their plan you'd find that it's very well-cited and fairly argued.


Not sure you should reject TCM categorically. That's where we got statins from. You dont even need an Rx to lower cholesterol, just use red yeast rice - works a charm and even has the same side effects.


And a in part a leukemia treatment, https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Arsenic_trioxide

and ephedrine..

I don't put much stock in the reasons behind why a lot of TCM works, but dismissing the whole thing out of hand is folly, because essentially you are saying plants have no medicinal value and massage is a waste of time.

I would push for more scientific rigour within TCM to come up with better models of why some of this stuff works instead, which to the best of my understanding isn't even a particularly controversial statement in many TCM circles.


Source for TCM playing a role in statin development? https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Discovery_and_development_of_s...


>> Source for TCM playing a role in statin development?

https://www.nature.com/articles/nrd1112#:~:text=In%201978%2C....

https://www.mayoclinic.org/drugs-supplements-red-yeast-rice/...

It's a shame the origin is missing from that wikipedia page.


artemisinin against malaria too


Why exactly does glucosamine only having a minor effect on pain, collagen degradation, joint function, etc. preclude it from having an effect on cancer outcomes?

Obviously this should be replicated and if not replicable, should be dismissed. But it's perplexing to dismiss it out of hand because its effects weren't strong enough for an array of other totally unrelated health outcomes.




Join us for AI Startup School this June 16-17 in San Francisco!

Guidelines | FAQ | Lists | API | Security | Legal | Apply to YC | Contact

Search: